Abstract
Visual perspective-taking (VPT) is a foundational skill for understanding the social world and emerges in early childhood. However, its developmental trajectory and associations with visual stimuli features and family socio-economic status (SES) remain underexplored. This study examined 254 preschool children aged 3 to 5 years (130 girls) using the “photographer task,” in which children identified the image representing a photographer’s view under varying stimuli conditions (complexity and angular differences). Primary caregivers provided demographic data via a questionnaire. Results indicated that children performed better with less complex stimuli and when the angular perspectives of the child and photographer aligned. Age differences were evident in most tasks, except those involving high complexity stimuli and conflicting angular perspectives. SES differences emerged, with high-SES children excelling in tasks without angular conflicts, while low-SES children demonstrated better performance in tasks with high complex stimuli with angular differences. These findings highlight the interplay between developmental, environmental, and task-specific factors in shaping early VPT and underscore the importance of tailoring early learning activities to account for these influences.




彩票大全软件:Similar content being viewed by others

彩票大全软件: The Broader Autism Phenotype and Visual Perception in Children

彩票大全软件: Working memory capacity, mental rotation, and visual perspective taking: A study of the developmental cascade hypothesis

彩票大全软件: Manipulating avatar age and gender in level-2 visual perspective taking
References
Anders, Y., Rossbach, H. G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehrl, S., & Von Maurice, J. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.003
Aram, D., & Biron, S. (2004). Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low SES preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(4), 588–610. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.10.003
Aslan, D., & K?ksal Akyol, A. (2020). Impact of an empathy training program on children’s perspective-taking abilities. Psychological Reports, 123(6), 2394–2409. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294119868785
Bai, H., & Wang, Y. (2021). Socioeconomic status and children’s perspective taking: A moderated mediation model. Paper presented at the 23rd National Conference on Psychology, Hohhot, Inner Mongolia, China. 2021:2. https://doi.org/10.26914/c.cnkihy.2021.040180
Baker, E. R., Jensen, C. J., Moeyaert, M., & Bordoff, S. (2020). Socioeconomic status and early childhood aggression: Moderation by theory of mind for relational, but not physical, aggression. Early Child Development and Care, 190(8), 1187–1201. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2018.1524379
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Harvard University Press.
Bryant, D. J., Tversky, B., & Franklin, N. (1992). Internal and external spatial frameworks for representing described scenes. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(1), 74–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(92)90006-J
Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(1), 55–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00862.x
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2014). Developing young children’s mathematical thinking and understanding. The Routledge International Handbook of Young Children’s Thinking and Understanding (pp. 331–344). Routledge.
Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 337–367. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.337
Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern families: Selected findings from the first decade of a prospective, longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(2), 361–373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00361.x
Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In Visual information processing (pp. 75–176). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-170150-5.50009-3
DeFlorio, L., & Beliakoff, A. (2015). Socioeconomic status and preschoolers’ mathematical knowledge: The contribution of home activities and parent beliefs. Early Education and Development, 26(3), 319–341. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.968239
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Ebert, S., Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., & Weinert, S. (2017). Links among parents’ mental state language, family socioeconomic status, and preschoolers’ theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 44, 32–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.005
Emen, M., & Aslan, D. (2019). The relationship between perspective taking skills and language development in preschool children. Journal of Education and Educational Development, 6(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.22555/joeed.v6i1.2159
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. F., Green, F. L., & Wilcox, S. A. (1980). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Effect of observer’s distance from target on perceptual clarity of target. Developmental Psychology, 16(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.16.1.10
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 99. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.99
Frick, A., M?hring, W., & Newcombe, N. S. (2014). Picturing perspectives: Development of perspective-taking abilities in 4-to 8-year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 88857. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00386
Grueneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). I know you don’t know I know… children use second-order false‐belief reasoning for peer coordination. Child Development, 86(1), 287–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12264
Guhn, M., Emerson, S. D., Mahdaviani, D., & Gadermann, A. M. (2020). Associations of birth factors and socio-economic status with indicators of early emotional development and mental health in childhood: A population-based linkage study. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 51(1), 80–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-019-00912-6
Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2009). Visual perspective taking impairment in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Cognition, 113(1), 37–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.007
Hirai, M., Sakurada, T., Ikeda, T., Monden, Y., Shimoizumi, H., & Yamagata, T. (2022). Developmental changes of the neural mechanisms underlying level 2 visual perspective-taking: A functional near‐infrared spectroscopy study. Developmental Psychobiology, 64(1), e22229. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22229
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Carr, T. H., & Nigg, J. T. (2002). Development of selective attention: Perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 363. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.3.363
Huttenlocher, J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2007). Using spatial categories to reason about location. The Emerging Spatial Mind, 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195189223.003.0001
Ikeda, A., Kanakogi, Y., & Hirai, M. (2022). Visual perspective-taking ability in 7-and 12-month-old infants. Plos One, 17(2), e0263653. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263653
Jin, X., Zhou, B., & Meng, F. (2019). Level 2 visual perspective-taking at age 3 and the corresponding effect of cooperation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(9), 1028. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.01028
Kessler, K., & Rutherford, H. (2010). The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 7810. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00213
Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2004). Genetic and environmental processes in young children’s resilience and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation. Child Development, 75(3), 651–668. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00699.x
Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105(3), 489–512. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.007
Martin, A. K., Perceval, G., Davies, I., Su, P., Huang, J., & Meinzer, M. (2019). Visual perspective taking in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(11), 2006. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000584
Masangkay, Z. S., McCluskey, K. A., McIntyre, C. W., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughn, B. E., & Flavell, J. H. (1974). The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others. Child Development, 357–366. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127956
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 603–613. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X55370
Moll, H., Meltzoff, A. N., Merzsch, K., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Taking versus confronting visual perspectives in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 49(4), 646. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028633
Nardini, M., Burgess, N., Breckenridge, K., & Atkinson, J. (2006). Differential developmental trajectories for egocentric, environmental and intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Cognition, 101(1), 153–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.09.005
Neppl, T. K., Senia, J. M., & Donnellan, M. B. (2016). Effects of economic hardship: Testing the family stress model over time. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000168
Newcombe, N., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/4395.001.0001
Obradovi?, J., Portilla, X. A., & Ballard, P. J. (2016). Biological sensitivity to family income: Differential effects on early executive functioning. Child Development, 87(2), 374–384. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12475
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285–308. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034226
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (2008). The psychology of the child. Basic books.
Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., Langdon, F. J., & Lunzer, J. L. (1956). La Représentation de L'espace Chez L'enfant. The Child's Conception of Space... Translated... by FJ Langdon & JL Lunzer. With Illustrations. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London; printed in Great Britain. https://doi.org/10.2307/3118882
Piaget, J., Trans, Ed, T. A., Brown, & Kaegi, C. E. (1981). Annual Reviews. https://nla.gov.au/nla.cat-vn1640221
Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., C?té, S., Cheng, B. H., & Keltner, D. (2010). Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020092
Pluck, G. (2021). Theory of mind ability and socioeconomic status, a study of street-connected children and adolescents in Ecuador. Psych, 3(2), 72–84. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych3020008
Poirel, N., Pineau, A., & Mellet, E. (2006). Implicit identification of irrelevant local objects interacts with global/local processing of hierarchical stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 122(3), 321–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.12.010
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the human brain. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11519-000
Rosen, M. L., Hagen, M. P., Lurie, L. A., Miles, Z. E., Sheridan, M. A., Meltzoff, A. N., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Cognitive stimulation as a mechanism linking socioeconomic status with executive function: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 91(4), e762–e779. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13315
Shi, B. G., & Shen, J. L. (2007). The relationships among family SES, intelligence, intrinsic motivation and creativity. Psychological Development and Education, 23(1), 30–34. https://doi.org/10.3969/j.issn.1001-4918.2007.01.006
Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75(3), 417–453. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075003417
Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month‐olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science, 10(2), 199–204. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00580.x
Song, H. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2008). Infants’ reasoning about others’ false perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1789. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013774
Spencer, N., Thanh, T. M., & Louise, S. (2013). Low income/socio-economic status in early childhood and physical health in later childhood/adolescence: A systematic review. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 17(3), 424–431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-012-1010-2
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18(7), 580–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
Surtees, A. D., & Apperly, I. A. (2012). Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. Child Development, 83(2), 452–460. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01730.x
Surtees, A. D., Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2012). Direct and indirect measures of level-2 perspective‐taking in children and adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02063.x
Surtees, A., Apperly, I., & Samson, D. (2013). The use of embodied self-rotation for visual and spatial perspective-taking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 698. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00698
Surtees, A., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. (2016). Unintentional perspective-taking calculates whether something is seen, but not how it is seen. Cognition, 148, 97–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.010
Tarighat, S., & Krott, A. (2021). Bilingualism enhances reported perspective taking in men, but not in women. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 679524. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679524
Tian, M., Luo, T., Ding, J., Wang, X., & Cheung, H. (2021). Spatial ability and theory of mind: A mediating role of visual perspective taking. Child Development, 92(4), 1590–1604. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13546
Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., & Umiltà, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4). https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.22.4.892
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of‐mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18(7), 600–606. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01946.x
Yaniv, I., & Shatz, M. (1990). Heuristics of reasoning and analogy in children’s visual perspective taking. Child Development, 61(5), 1491–1501. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02877.x
Author information
彩票大全软件:Authors and Affiliations
彩票大全软件:Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
彩票大全软件:Competing interest
We declare that there are no competing interests in this study.
Additional information
彩票大全软件:Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
Reprints and permissions
About this article
彩票大全软件:Cite this article
Liang, J., Sun, J., Xu, X. et al. Early Development of Visual Perspective-Taking: Its Associations with Stimuli Feature, Child Age, and Family Socio-Economic Status. Early Childhood Educ J (2025). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-025-01866-2
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-025-01866-2